Christopher Taylor Bird Nature Wildlife Mammal Photography
bird photography
GALLERIES > BIRDS > PASSERIFORMES > EMBERIZIDAE > RUFOUS-COLLARED SPARROW [Zonotrichia capensis]


Rufous-collared Sparrow Image @ Kiwifoto.com
 
 
Location: Ushuaia, Argentina
GPS: -54.8S, -68.6W, elev=21' MAP
Date: January 5, 2010
ID : 7C2V5745 [3888 x 2592]

Rufous-collared Sparrow Picture @ Kiwifoto.com
 
 
Location: Ushuaia, Argentina
GPS: -54.8S, -68.6W, elev=21' MAP
Date: January 5, 2010
ID : 7C2V6378 [3888 x 2592]

bird photography

Rufous-collared Sparrow (juvenile)
 
 
Location: Ushuaia, Argentina
GPS: -54.8S, -68.6W, elev=21' MAP
Date: January 5, 2010
ID : 7C2V6492 [3888 x 2592]

bird photography

SPECIES INFO

The Rufous-collared Sparrow, Zonotrichia capensis, is an American sparrow which breeds in highlands from the extreme southeast of Mexico to Tierra del Fuego, and on the island of Hispaniola. It is famous for its diverse vocalizations, which have been intensely studied for the last decades.

Description and ecology

The Rufous-collared Sparrow is 13.5-15 cm long and weighs 20-25 g. The adult has a stubby grey bill and a grey head with broad black stripes on the crown sides and thinner stripes through the eye and below the cheeks. The nape and breast sides are rufous and the upperpart are black-streaked buff-brown. There are two white wing bars. The throat is white, and the underparts are off-white, becoming brown on the flanks and with a black breast patch.

Young birds have a duller, indistinct head pattern, with brown stripes and a buff ground colour. They lack the rufous collar, and have streaked underparts.

There are between 25 and 29 subspecies. In general, the smaller forms occur in coastal mountains, intermediate birds in the Andes, and large, darker, forms breed on the tepuis. The largest tepui subspecies, Z. c. perezchincillae has grey underparts, and the rufous collar extends as a black band of freckles across the breast. This form might be separable as a distinct species.

This abundant bird is found typically at altitudes between 600 and 4000m, locally lower, in open areas including cultivation, gardens, parks and scrubby second growth. It copes well with urban and suburban environments, but is absent from the Amazon basin. Explaining the presence of this species in the island of Hispaniola, while absent from the rest of the Caribbean basin, may be similar to the theory proposed for the Loxia megaplaga, in which the bird was present in the region during the much cooler climes of the last ice age, but was left marooned in the tall Hispaniolan mountains once warming began. The local name in the Dominican Republic for this bird is "cigüita de Constanza".

The nest, built by the female, is a cup of plant material lined with fine grasses. It is constructed in matted vegetation on the ground, low in a tree or bush, or in a niche in a wall. The female lays two or three brown-blotched greenish-blue eggs, which she incubates for 12-14 days. The male helps in feeding the chicks.

The Rufous-collared Sparrow feeds on the ground on seeds, fallen grain, insects and spiders. It is usually in pairs which hold small territories or in small flocks. It is tame and approachable.

Vocalizations Rufous-collared Sparrow

The Rufous-collared Sparrow has extensive geographical variation in its vocalisations (see section below), but calls include a sharp tsip. The male's song, given from a low perch, typically includes slurred whistles with or without a final trill, tee-teeooo, e'e'e'e'e, or teeooo, teeeee.

For subtropical/temperate populations in Argentina, except as noted:

Songs are typically two-part: an introductory phrase (termed the theme, following Nottebohm's original description of the song) of 2 to 4 pure tone whistles, which are flat, rising, falling, or rising then falling in pitch, followed by a terminal trill, composed of several to many identical (or nearly so) elements. There is a high degree of stereotypy of song within individuals, both within and among seasons. The trill rate is locally very consistent, but varies greatly among populations, with inter-element interval ranging from 12msec. to 400+ msec.

In some areas (in arid parts of N.W. Argentina, in eastern Patagonia, and in certain sites in Costa Rica) there is (often or always) no terminal trill, and the song comprises whistles only. A few individuals in some few localities (so far only in montane grasslands) show two terminal trills, the first rapid, the second substantially slower.

Song Measures: Main accounts of this are in Nottebohm 1969, King 1972, and Handford & Lougheed 1991. Songs typically ~2-2.5 seconds in duration. The whistled theme notes are each ~0.25-0.5 sec in duration, and are 2-3 in number in typical songs (from a sample of 1764 individuals, mean # notes/song = 2.87: 1-note themes - 0.5%; 2-note - 27.6%; 3-note - 58%; 4-note - 13%; 5-note - 0.8%; 7-note - 0.1%). These notes are either 1) level, 2) rising, 3) falling, or 4) rising then falling in pitch. Absolute abundance of these note types: 1) - 15.9%; 2) - 32.0%; 3) - 39.8%; 4) - 11.4%. On a notes per song basis, note-type frequency is: 1) - 0.46; 2) - 0.92; 3) - 1.14; 4) - 0.32. Most of the energy in these notes lies between 4 and 6 kHz, with a range of 2.27 - 8.8kHz. The terminal trill comprises several to many near-identical elements, which are descending frequency sweeps, with a maximum frequency of 3.8 - 8.7kHz and a minimum frequency of 2.4 - 4.9kHz. See further details under Geographic Variation.

Singing Behaviour: Individuals will sing for up to 30 minutes at a time, though usually 2-5 minutes, and countersinging is evident, though not well-studied. Singing-rate is regular, and usually 10-12 per minute. Typically from some elevated point, where available - a large rock, bush, etc. In open scrub and grassland, will sing from stem-tops. In suburban situations, will sing from low branches of trees, walls, sheds, etc. Individuals have "favourite" singing points, used repeatedly both within and among seasons. Flight songs have been recorded in migrating groups; these songs seem to be longer and more complex than typical territorial songs, and resemble night songs. Night-singing is recorded, though it is rare and unpredictable. Anecdotal evidence suggests that it may relate to stress. Night songs are typically unlike daytime songs, being longer and more complex. Though there is a peak of singing activity near dawn, chingolos will sing strongly, if not persistently, at almost any time of day during the main season (September to January), except when mid-day temperatures are much above 30¡C. There is a slight resurgence of activity in the evening.

Song Repertoires: In the best-studied populations, in n.w. Argentina, songs appear highly stereotyped, with the great majority of individuals showing a single, song. There is good evidence that this song does not change among years, at least after first breeding. However, there is evidence from Ecuador that tropical populations show individual repertoires of up to 7 diverse song types.

Sex Differences: Females apparently do not sing, though this is not known with certainty.

Seasonal Variation: Very little studied. There is unpublished evidence that, in Patagonian populations, in the early season, individuals may sing more than one song, but this phenomenon seems to disappear by the time the breeding season is properly under way.

Ontogeny: So far as is known (some Ph.D. thesis studies of P. Tubaro), seems to be very similar to Z. leucophrys.

Vocal dialects

This ecologically-catholic neotropical songbird provides perhaps one of the clearest and most widely-distributed habitat-related dialect systems. The geographic variation in the song of this species became apparent over 30 years ago with Nottebohm's study in subtropical and temperate Argentina. He interpreted his findings largely in the context provided by Marler & Tamura, that is, he suggested that these dialects perhaps serve to enhance the genetic integrity of local populations. The first direct investigation of this possibility, while providing no support for what came to be called the Genetic Adaptation Hypothesis (GAH), showed that the spatial organisation of song variation was very closely associated with the distribution of distinct habitat types. Moreover, the structural characteristics of the dialect variable (trill interval) showed variation largely consistent with the interspecific acoustic patterns described by Morton, that is, in general, the trill interval varied from short (~50 msec; rapid trills) in open grasslands to long (1-200 msec; slow whistles) in woodlands and forests.

This ecological dimension was explored further by Handford and students in the highly diverse habitats of northwestern Argentina. They showed that the ecological ordering of dialect variation over a huge geographical space (1200 x 350 km) and across a dramatic sweep of structurally-distinct habitats (puna scrub, grassland, desert scrub, thorn woodland, and drought-deciduous forest (see Figure) was largely consistent with the previously-established picture. This work also demonstrated that these spatial patterns show temporal stability of at least 20 years (now known to exceed 30 years), and stability on the order of centuries is implied by the persistence of certain habitat dialects long after the native vegetation has been removed by agriculture. This massive demonstration of acoustically rational habitat-based song variation strongly supports what is now known as the Acoustic Adaptation Hypothesis. However, the work also provided a basis for a final evaluation of the GAH on a similar geographical scale. This study showed that the substantial genetic variation shown by the species is organised largely by distance; dialect songs impose no further structure: it seems that for this species the GAH has no explanatory value.

The most recent work on this species confirms that the clear ecological segregation of acoustically rational vocal dialects in Argentina extends from 22ºS at the Bolivian border south to 42ºS in northern Patagonia. Across this vast space, the greatest song diversity is concentrated in the vegetationally-diverse north west; in the ecologically more uniform central and southern regions, great song uniformity is encountered; finally, island habitats, such as montane grasslands, are represented by repeated islands of the specific song dialect. Other recent work suggests, however, that tropical population (Ecuador) do not show this pattern: instead, individuals show repertoires (from 1-7 trill-types; mean = ~4) and local populations can show nearly as much trill variation as is known from all Argentina.





HOME · ABOUT ME · GALLERY · STOCKLIST · VIDEO · SEARCH · PRESS · CONTACT · BLOG · NEW STUFF
nature photography
All images and video © Copyright 2006-2024 Christopher Taylor, Content and maps by their respective owner. All rights reserved.
nature photography